StructureActivity Relations in Olfaction - American Chemical Society

proposes that there is a further region which recognizes this moiety and accounts for ..... tivity from bundles of receptor nerve fibers in the rat. T...
5 downloads 0 Views 2MB Size
12 Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction From Single Cell to Behavior—The Comparative Approach DAVID G. MOULTON

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

Veterans Administration Medical Center and Department of Physiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadephia, PA 19104

Odorants excite receptor cells presumably by interacting with the hypothetical receptor sites. But to reach these sites, odorants must first be transported from a point at which their concentration is known, across the liquid secretions (mucus) lining the surface nasal of nasal airways, through the mucus/air phase boundary and possible to the base of the mucociliary blanket. The mucus is rich in microproteins, Na ions and pigmented granules. Within the mucus, odorant molecules may partition between different liquid phases. Thus separate subsets of physiochemical factors govern stages of transport and odorant-receptor interaction. Consequently, the verbal response - the indicator used in human psychophysical studies of structure-activity relations - reflects the end product of events whose separate contributions are unknown. What is needed in interpreting such data, is a means of segregating and manipulating separate phases of the process or of components of the chemosensory system and assessing their relative influences on the final measured response. To do so we must turn to animal studies. Thus, in the appropriate preparation, it is possible to eliminate certain transport factors; to employ an aqueous rather than a vapor phase to transport odorants to the olfactory surface; to study separately the response characteristics of subpopulations of receptors differing in their structure-activity relations (including the separate contributions of the olfactory receptors and the highly chemosensitive endings of the trigeminal nerve in the nasal mucosa), and to take advantage of the various anatomical and functional features peculiar to specific animal groups such as the extreme absolute sensitivity to certain odorants shown by the dog. The power of the comparative approach to structure-activity relations can be illustrated with selected examples drawn from electrophysiological studies in fish, amphibians and mammals; and behavioral studies in mammals. What follows is a selective and not a comprehensive review of relevant comparative research. +

This chapter not subject to U.S. copyright. Published 1981 American Chemical Society

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

212

ODOR QUALITY AND CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

Electrophysiological studies i n f i s h The most extensive s t u d i e s on s t r u c t u r e - a c t i v i t y r e l a t i o n s i n o l f a c t i o n - apart from those on humans - have been on f i s h . This i n t e r e s t r e l a t e s p a r t l y to the commercial importance of t h i s group. But there are advantages i n d e l i v e r i n g odorants i n the aqueous phase: s o r p t i o n onto the f l u i d s e c r e t i o n s (mucus) covering the o l f a c t o r y surface i s l i k e l y to be l e s s than occurs with gaseous odorants and the odorant p a r t i t i o n c o e f f i c i e n t f o r water/ mucus w i l l be c l o s e r to one than would be the case f o r air/mucus. For example, carvone i s s t r o n g l y sorbed anteromedially when flowed i n the vapor phase over the frog's o l f a c t o r y epithelium and has a r e l a t i v e l y long r e t e n t i o n time on t h i s t i s s u e (_1,2) . The same compound i n the aqueous phase (Ringer's s o l u t i o n ) was flowed over the frog's o l f a c t o r y o l f a c t o r y epithelium p r i o r to washing with t r i t i a t e d N-ethylmaleimide (NEM - a group s p e c i f i c p r o t e i n reagent). S i t e s protected by carvone from a t t a c k by NEM were subsequently found d i s t r i b u t e d evenly across the epithelium. This suggests that s o r p t i v e e f f e c t s do not c o n t r o l odorant d i s t r i b u t i o n i n the aqueous phase ( 3 ) . I t i s true that most of the compounds that normally e x c i t e the o l f a c t o r y organ i n f i s h d i f f e r from those to which a i r breathing vertebrates are exposed, but there i s no evidence that the b a s i c transduction mechanisms i n a i r and water d i f f e r s i g n i f i c a n t ly. I t i s known, f o r example, that the same odorants d e l i v e r e d i n the aqueous phase, a r e as e f f e c t i v e as when d e l i v e r e d i n the vapor phase as judged by the slow voltage s h i f t recorded when a macroelectrode t i p was p o s i t i o n e d i n the nasal c a v i t y of a box t u r t l e during odorant s t i m u l a t i o n (k_) . A f u r t h e r advantage i n using f i s h i s anatomical. In a i r breathing vertebrates the o l f a c t o r y chamber extends from the r e s p i r a t o r y airway; i n most f i s h , however, i t i s a separate organ divorced from r e s p i r a t o r y f u n c t i o n s . This f e a t u r e , and the presence of an aqueous medium, allows us to p l a c e a c o n d u c t i v i t y e l e c t r o d e at the i n l e t and one at the o u t l e t of the nasal chamber. I f e l e c t r o l y t e s are used as odorants, t h e i r a r r i v a l and departure from the chamber can then be measured by c o n d u c t i v i t y changes. Since c o n d u c t i v i t y i s p r o p o r t i o n a l to concentration we can speci f y odorant concentration, w i t h i n known l i m i t s , c l o s e to the r e ceptors - something which cannot be done with the i n t a c t n a s a l chamber i n a i r - b r e a t h i n g v e r t e b r a t e s . I t i s a l s o p o s s i b l e to d e l i v e r the odorant i n a way that c l o s e l y i m i t a t e s that i n which i t normally a r r i v e s ( 5 ) . Among the most e f f e c t i v e o l f a c t o r y s t i m u l i f o r f i s h are amino a c i d s . For example, thresholds of 10" have been reported f o r L-glutamine i n the Conger e e l (6) and of 3.2 x 10" i n the A t l a n t i c salmon (7) and c a t f i s h (8). This s e n s i t i v i t y i s presumably r e l a t e d to the widespread d i s t r i b u t i o n of amino acids i n

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

MOULTON

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

213

f i s h s k i n e x t r a c t s , which e l i c i t f r i g h t and alarm r e a c t i o n s i n other f i s h of the same species (e.g. 9 ) ; i n mammalian s k i n , which a c t as a r e p e l l a n t to salmon (10,-13); and i n substances that a t t r a c t or e l i c i t feeding behavior i n f i s h (14,15). I t i s not s u r p r i s i n g , t h e r e f o r e , that most s t r u c t u r e - a c t i v i t y s t u d i e s on f i s h o l f a c t i o n have centered on amino a c i d s . Despite the range of species that have been i n v e s t i g a t e d , the v a r i e t y of techniques used and the presence of some species d i f f e r e n c e s i n response (see, f o r example, 6), there i s considerable agreement between workers on the f a c t o r s that govern neural r e s p o n s e , i r r e s p e c t i v e of whether a c t i v i t y i s recorded at a p e r i pheral or higher l e v e l (16-19,6). For example, ^-amino acids e l i c i t the maximum responses, and the most e f f e c t i v e member of a c h i r a l p a i r i s the L-isomer. (Of these, L-glutamine or La l a n i n e are the most powerful s t i m u l i f o r the majority of species so f a r t e s t e d , but not f o r a l l ) . An amino a c i d c o n s i s t s of an asymmetrical carbon center surrounded by four f u n c t i o n a l groups: (1) ^-amino (2) primarycarboxyl (3) ^-hydrogen and (4) a s i d e chain, R:

Response amplitudes can be reduced by s u b s t i t u t i n g other funct i o n a l groups (-H, -CHo, -OH) f o r the -amino group; by methyla t i o n or a c e t y l a t i o n or the ^-amino moiety; by s u b s t i t u t i o n of the ^-hydrogen; o r , i n some cases, at l e a s t , by r e p l a c i n g the primary-carboxyl group. The most e f f e c t i v e amino acids are g e n e r a l l y those with 5-6 carbon atoms and with l i n e a r and uncharged s i d e chains. Amida t i o n g r e a t l y increases the e f f e c t i v e n e s s of a s p a r t i c and g l u t amic a c i d , and s u l f u r - c o n t a i n i n g amino acids are a l s o p a r t i c u l a r l y strong e x c i t a n t s . However, Caprio (19) has concluded t h a t , i n general, the S atom may be equivalent to another C atom i n the chain. The above i n t e r p r e t a t i o n s of the data do not consider the a l t e r n a t i v e i m p l i c a t i o n s of a m u l t i p l e receptor s i t e model f o r the odorant-receptor i n t e r a c t i o n . In such a model the response e l i c i t e d by a l i g a n d r e s u l t s from the simultaneous b i n d i n g of s e v e r a l groups rather than one. Thus i f one group i s modified i t may a l t e r the odorant molecule i n such a way that i t no longer binds to other s i t e s c o n t r i b u t i n g to the response. Hara (17) has proposed a model of the amino a c i d receptor s i t e c o n s i s t i n g of two charged s u b s i t e s , one c a t i o n i c and one a n i o n i c , capable of i n t e r a c t i n g with i o n i z e d ^-amino and p r i mary-carboxyl groups of amino a c i d molecules. He assumes that the L-isomers have more ready access to the receptor and accounts oc

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

214

ODOR QUALITY AND

CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

f o r t h i s by p o s t u l a t i n g that the two subsites are arranged around the t h i r d c e n t r a l s u b s i t e i n such a way that i t accommodates the ^-hydrogen atom of an amino a c i d molecules. Since the fourth rc-amino moiety g r e a t l y i n f l u e n c e s s t i m u l a t i n g e f f e c t i v e n e s s he proposes that there i s a f u r t h e r region which recognizes t h i s moiety and accounts f o r d i s c r i m a t i n g amino-acid q u a l i t y . Caprio (19), however, has argued that the b i n d i n g of the primary c a r boxyl group may not be p r i m a r i l y i o n i c . In the rainbow t r o u t , o l f a c t o r y bulb neurones seem to d i s criminate between various chemical s t i m u l i having only s l i g h t l y d i s s i m i l a r molecular s t r u c t u r e s and conformations. In f a c t , s e v e r a l c e l l s , i n one study, gave opposite responses to members of enantiomeric p a i r s of amino a c i d s : the L-isomer g e n e r a l l y e x c i t e d while the D-isomer i n h i b i t e d the c e l l (18). There are three problems i n p a r t i c u l a r that complicate i n t e r p r e t a t i o n of much of the data on s t r u c t u r e - a c t i v i t y r e l a t i o n s i n o l f a c t i o n . F i r s t , the d i f f e r e n t techniques used o f t e n y i e l d data that are not s t r i c t l y comparable. Recordings from a s i n g l e or a few receptors, f o r example, are more r e l i a b l e i n d i c a t o r s of the odorant-receptor i n t e r a c t i o n than are recordings of the massed a c t i o n of many neural elements i n the o l f a c t o r y bulb. Thus discrepancies among r e s u l t s are to be expected. Second, many workers record without regard to the existence of topographic d i f f e r e n c e s i n the s e n s i t i v i t y of the system to d i f f e r e n t odorants. For example, DtJving et a l (20) showed that b i l e acids e l i c i t e d responses ( i n the o l f a c t o r y bulb of chars and g r a y l i n g s ) which d i f f e r e d s p a t i a l l y from those of two amino a c i d s . A t h i r d d i f f i c u l t y i s that many workers i n v e s t i g a t e the r e sponse to only one concentration of each odorant. But i t i s w e l l known that some odorants can increase neural a c t i v i t y at low concentrations and suppress i t at higher concentrations (21,20,5). This r a i s e s the p o s s i b i l i t y that the r e l a t i v e s t i m u l a t i n g e f f e c tiveness of a group of odorants e s t a b l i s h e d at one concentration, i s not the same as that e x i s t i n g at another l e v e l . The point i s w e l l i l l u s t r a t e d i n a study by Meredith (22,_23,j>) . The aim was to e s t a b l i s h and analyse response s i m i l a r i t i e s of s i n g l e bulbar neurones i n the g o l d f i s h when stimulated s u c c e s s i v e l y by seven amino acids - each a c i d being presented i n not one, but two d i f f e r e n t concentrations. The compounds used, t h e i r s t r u c t u r e s and c e r t a i n p h y s i c a l p r o p e r t i e s are shown i n Table I. Response s i m i l a r i t y was measured by c o r r e l a t i n g temporal patterns of c e l l f i r i n g r a t e (rather than maximum f i r i n g r a t e which was l e s s c h a r a c t e r i s t i c of odor type and concentration) using the Spearman Rank Order C o r r e l a t i o n (p). (A mean s i m i l a r i t y measure f o r a given stimulus p a i r was determined by f i n d i n g the average f i r i n g r a t e across a l l u n i t s t e s t e d . Guttman-Lingoes nonmetric mulitdimensional s c a l i n g procedure was a p p l i e d to the matrix representing a l l p a i r s of mean s i m i l a r i t y measures r e s u l t ing i n the arrangements i n F i g u r e ( 1 ) . In these p l o t s the rank order of distances between points i s the inverse of the rank

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

MOULTON

215

Table I.

Physico-chemical Constants and Structures of Amino Acids Substance MW Symbol Glycine

75.1 G

Structure

Substance M W Symbol

Structure

H-CHC0 H

CH -CH S0 H I NH

2

2

I

NH

Taurine 125.1 T 2

2

3

2

Alanine

89.1 A

CH -CHC0 H 3

2

NH /3-alanine 89.1

B

105.1 S

165.2

P

Pentanoic acid 11021 —

-Butanol

STRUCTURE

H

'

H

0 CHjCh CH CH -C-OH 2

2

2

- .• I Insol. si. sol. sol. very sol. miscible

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

Dimethyl sulfide 62 | 37 |

u

Trimethylamine 59 3 | | ^Heptanal

1114 1531 M 0

CHj

1

CH,SCHj

CHJNCHJ

CH CH CH CH CH CH -C-H 3

2

2

2

2

2

0 Phenylethanone 120 202|

^ 2-Pro pa none

COCH

3

CH3-C-CH3

Compound 100 150^^/3-lonone Molecular weight-^ / / / Boiling point ' / / Solubility in water '/ Solubility in alcohol '



192 |140|_J a-Terpineol

11541220l '• CH,

i I

B

^

O

0

6



58 |56^^JCyclopenianone| 84 1311

H

JCOH 3 "XH C

3

insol. si. sol. sol. very sol. miscible

Geraniol

154 2301

il521285J ki Isoeugenol

^ Vanillin

1164[134| L

^^CH OH 2

HjC

?

CHj

CHO

H

CH=CH-CH

3

3-Phenyl _ |148 Napthalena |196| - | L. - propanoic acid ' ..^•M

Cantharadin 0

OCH,

?

128 218|

8

1

5

4

^

00

I

CH, CH 3

BOCC-C- "

N 0

6

H

6

3

N

Figure 2. Structures and selected properties of odorants used to stimulate olfactory epithelium of tiger salamander. Odorants are classified according to their relative effectiveness in stimulating different epithelial regions (33).

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

Posterior stimulants

_

100 150 n-Octane Compound Molecular Weight^ / / / Boiling Point / / / Solubility in water '/ Solubility in alcohol ' • insol.

U

i

B

si. sol.

221

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

MOULTON

1

s o l . very sol miscible

Hexachloro-

114 126

|l02|l02| !• D-Limonene

136 178

Uniform stimulants H Cyclohexanone 98

1 5 5

BM

O ^n-Pentyl acetate 130 149

CH

J|

3

CHsCOOCsHu

CI3CCCI3

HC 3

a -Pinene

136 1561 gJD-Camphor

CH

2

152 204 !• n-Decyl acetate 1230 244

CH

3

CHj CH C0 (CH ) CH 3

Q

CH

2

2

9

3

^r^-CH,

3

Figure 2, Continued

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

u

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

222

ODOR QUALITY AND

CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

p e n t y l a c e t a t e g e n e r a l l y s t i m u l a t e s maximally along a d i s t i n c t r i d g e extending a n t e r i o r l y f o r about 2mm from the d-limonene s e n s i t i v e zone located p o s t e r i o r l y . F o c i f o r peak s e n s i t i v i t y to eugenol and iso-eugenol are a l s o c l e a r l y segregated (34). In t h i s study odorants were d e l i v e r e d by way of a microp i p e t t e p o s i t i o n e d l e s s than a mm from the o l f a c t o r y s u r f a c e (a m o d i f i c a t i o n of a technique described by Kauer and Moulton (35)) . This e l i m i n a t e s any p o s s i b i l i t y that the d i f f e r e n c e s reported r e s u l t e d from a d i f f e r e n t i a l s o r p t i o n of odorant molecules by the mucus. In the behaving animal, however, odorants flow over the mucus a n t e r i o r l y to p o s t e r i o r l y . Because d i f f e r e n t odorants have d i f f e r e n t mean r e l a t i v e r e t e n t i o n times on the o l f a c t o r y mucosa O ,36) they may c r e a t e d i f f e r e n t gradients of e x c i t a t i o n across i t (except f o r those with r e l a t i v e l y short mean r e t e n t i o n t i m e s ) . To what extent such f a c t o r s c o n t r i b u t e to the o v e r a l l s p a t i a l p a t t e r n of e x c i t a t i o n i s not yet c l e a r (37). Cone entra t ion-response r e l a t ions Despite the apparently widespread conformity of many sensory f u n c t i o n s to the Weber-Fechner or Steven's Power Laws, the r e l a t i o n s between stimulus i n t e n s i t y and response magnitude can sometimes be more complex. For example, d i s c o n t i n u i t i e s i n t h i s r e l a t i o n are a s s o c i a t e d with dual somesthetic receptor f u n c t i o n s (38) and with dual f u n c t i o n s of a s i n g l e receptor type i n the r e t i n a (39). Should such d e v i a t i o n s occur i n o l f a c t o r y functions, they may not have been i d e n t i f i e d i n many s t u d i e s because of the very assumption that a simple r e l a t i o n must e x i s t between conc e n t r a t i o n and response. T h i s assumption determines the concent r a t i o n s at which response measures are made - a number which may be inadequate to r e v e a l any d e v i a t i o n s from a simple r e l a t i o n . A l t e r n a t i v e l y , i f they do appear, they may be dismissed as s t a t i s t i c a l l y i n s i g n i f i c a n t a b e r r a t i o n s . Yet, even i f a curve r e f l e c t e d only l i g a n d b i n d i n g i t i s u n l i k e l y to be simple. In a v a r i e t y of n e u r a l and other t i s s u e s , b i n d i n g curves are i n f l u enced by v a r i o u s froms of c o o p e r a t i v i t y (binding, e f f e c t and i n t e r m o l e c u a l r c o o p e r a t i v i t y ) . For example, i n b i n d i n g coopera t i v i t y the presence of l i g a n d molecules already bound can a l t e r the a f f i n i t y of the r e c e p t o r f o r a d d i t i o n a l l i g a n d b i n d i n g (40). But i n f a c t , f u r t h e r complexity may be imposed by events preceding and succeeding odorant b i n d i n g . The most s i g n i f i c a n t of these are transport f a c t o r s and n o n l i n e a r transform f u n c t i o n s w i t h i n the c e n t r a l nervous system ( i n the case of measures taken at bulbar or higher l e v e l s ) . I t has a l s o been suggested that enzymes, which are probably present i n the mucus, may degrade odorant molecules d i f f u s i n g towards b i n d i n g s i t e s ( N i c o l l i n i 41). Thus as G e t c h e l l and G e t c h e l l (42) have noted, p e n t y l acetate may degrade to pentanol and a c e t i c a c i d . These events could f u r t h e r d i s t o r t the concentration-response curve. In view of these f a c t o r s i t i s not s u r p r i z i n g that s i n g l e

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

MOULTON

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

223

c e l l s i n the g o l d f i s h o l f a c t o r y bulb y i e l d curves w i t h a v a r i e t y of shapes. Some, f o r example, show monotonic response f u n c t i o n s while others show i n i t i a l l y i n c r e a s i n g and then decreasing f i r i n g r a t e s as c o n c e n t r a t i o n i s increased ( F i g . 3 ) . Some non-monotonic response f u n c t i o n s were a l s o recorded i n f r o g and salamander o l f a c t o r y r e c e p t o r s ( 4 3 , 4 4 ) . The average of c o n c e n t r a t i o n responsecurves showing a v a r i e t y of forms, such as those i n F i g . 3 , could be a r e l a t i v e l y complex f u n c t i o n . But whatever the reason, f o r some odorants at l e a s t , curves derived from l a r g e populations of receptors do show marked notches. They appear i n data generated both e l e c t r o p h y s i o l o g i c a l l y and p s y c h o p h y s i c a l ^ ( F i g . 4 ) . In the case of the psychophysical curve f o r °=-ionone seen i n data from dogs, the notch i s h i g h l y s i g n i f i c a n t s t a t i s t i c a l l y and d i v i d e s the curve i n t o a slowly descending upper limb, best f i t t e d by a p a r a b o l i c f u n c t i o n , and a r a p i d l y descending lower limb, best f i t t e d by a cubic f u n c t i o n ( F i g . 5 ) . In an homologous s e r i e s of a l i p h a t i c acetates the p o s i t i o n of t h i s notch on the curve ascends w i t h i n c r e a s i n g chain l e n g t h , and i t has been suggested that the notch may r e f l e c t the independent c o n t r i b u t i o n s of two types of receptors - the response of one, c o n t r o l l i n g the lower limb of the curve, and that of the other, c o n t r o l l i n g the form of the upper limb ( 2 4 ) . An a l t e r n a t i v e e x p l a n a t i o n , however, i s that the a f f i n i t y of a s i n g l e type of s i t e f o r the odorant changes as a c r i t i c a l c o n c e n t r a t i o n i s reached. The form of the concentration-response curve o f f e r s a potent i a l approach to grouping odorants, and Mathews (48) has made a promising s t a r t i n t h i s d i r e c t i o n . He recorded the averaged act i v i t y from bundles of r e c e p t o r nerve f i b e r s i n the r a t . The seven odorants he tested f e l l i n t o three groups according to the slope and form of the curves that they e l i c i t e d . Members of the f i r s t group were n-pentyl a c e t a t e and two compounds with a pepperm i n i t y odor: menthone and 2-sec b u t y l hexanone. T h e i r curves showed c l e a r notches and a c c e l e r a t e d n e g a t i v e l y towards t h e i r asymptotes. The second group contained l i n a l o o l and dimethyl benzyl carbonyl a c e t a t e - compounds w i t h a f l o r a l odor and p o s i t i v e l y a c c e l e r a t i n g curves. In the t h i r d group were camphor and i s o - b o r n e o l , both w i t h a camphoraceous odor and a curve c o n s i s t ing of a lower n e g a t i v e l y a c c e l e r a t i n g limb and a l i n e a r upper limb. Further evidence that the slope of the concentration-response curve may be r e l a t e d i n a p r e d i c t a b l e way to the physiochemical p r o p e r t i e s of the odorant molecule comes from a study of the r e l a t i v e d e t e c t a b i l i t y of members of an homologous s e r i e s of a l i p h a t i c acetates ( 4 9 ) . P r o b i t r e g r e s s i o n l i n e s were f i r s t derived from the concentration-response data f o r each member of the s e r i e s . The slopes of these l i n e s , when p l o t t e d a g a i n s t l o g carbon chain l e n g t h , y i e l d e d an approximately l i n e a r r e l a t i o n . One consequence of the r e l a t i o n i s that each of s e v e r a l p r o b i t r e g r e s s i o n l i n e s i n t e r c e p t s one or more other l i n e s . Thus the r e l a t i v e e f f e c t i v e n e s s of these compounds depends on the p e r f o r -

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

224

ODOR QUALITY AND CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

I

1

6

10" M

lO'^M

I 2

l(f M Journal of General Physiology

Figure 3. Concentration-response relations of J1 units to glycine. Magnitude of response, measured as the normalized average firing rate for the rise and plateau phases of the stimulus, is plotted on the ordinate (5).

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

MOULTON

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

oLi -2.0

1

1

-1.5

-1.0

i -0.5

I _ I 0.0

i 0.5

i 1.0

225

lo 1.5

Pentyl acetate (log Mg/I)

Figure 4. Comparison of concentration-response functions for amyl acetate derived from psychophysical and electrophysiological measures of response. The partially overlapping curves are for the rat—one was generated by rats performing on an odor choice apparatus (24) while the other reflects the massed responses of receptor nerve bundles (45). The remaining curve is the averaged multiunit activity of the rabbit olfactory bulb (46).

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

226

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

ODOR QUALITY AND CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

1

1 9

i

i

8

i

7

i

6

i—

5

4

3

10" 10" 10" 10" 10" 10~ 10" FRACTION OF SATURATED VAPOR AT 23°

309

8

7

6

5

4

10" 10 10" 1(T 10" 1(T FRACTION OF SATURATED VAPOR

3

1(T

Journal of Comparative Physiology Figure 5. (a) Performance of 4 dogs in detecting a-ionone in the vapor phase (dog no. 1 (%—%); 2 (O—O); 3 (A—A); 4 (A—A)); (b) least-squares curve fits to the data shown in (a), assuming the response function is the sum of two distinct processes (dog no. 1 ( ); 2( ); 3( ); 4 (— • — •)) (Al).

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

12.

MOULTON

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

227

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

mance l e v e l that i s chosen as a b a s i s f o r comparison. For example, i f a 50% c o r r e c t response score i s taken as the c r i t e r i o n (chance being 50% c o r r e c t ) , the r e l a t i o n between performance and response i s l i n e a r . I f , however, an 85% c o r r e c t c r i t e r i o n i s chosen a p a r t i a l l y c u r v i l i n e a r r e l a t i o n emerges (49). The dependence o f response s i m i l a r i t i e s (determined e l e c t r o p h y s i o l o g i c a l l y ) on c o n c e n t r a t i o n was discussed above i n r e l a t i o n to a group of amino a c i d s . Thus the physicochemical p r o p e r t i e s o f an odorant that c o n t r o l i t s r e l a t i v e s t i m u l a t i n g e f f e c t i v e n e s s a t one c o n c e n t r a t i o n are not n e c e s s a r i l y those c o n t r o l l i n g e f f e c t i v e ness a t another c o n c e n t r a t i o n . Acknowled %ment s We thank Dr. Michael Meredith f o r permission to i n c l u d e f i g u r e s and data from a p r e s e n t a t i o n and f o r c r i t i c a l l y reading the manuscript. We a l s o thank Dr. Susan Schiffman f o r permission to i n c l u d e the r e s u l t s of a multidimensional a n a l y s i s of the data and f o r h e l p f u l d i s c u s s i o n and comments on the manuscript. Part of the work described here was supported by NIH grant No. 5 R01 NS 10617-04 and AFOSR grant No. 77-3162.

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

228

ODOR QUALITY AND CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

Literature Cited 1. Mozell, M.M. Evidence for a chromatographic model of olfaction. J. of Gen. Physiol., 1970, 46-63. 2. Mozell, M.M. and Jagodowicz, M. Chromatographic separation of odorants by the nose: retention times measured across in vivo olfactory mucosa. Science, 1973, 1247-1249. 3. Nathan, M.H. The localization of sulfhydryl groups and maping of specific olfactory receptor sites in the frog olfactory epithelium. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Michigan, 1979. 4. Tucker, D. and Shibuya, S. A physiologic and pharmacologic study of olfactory receptors. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium of Quantitative Biology, 1965, 30, 207-215. 5. Meredith, M. and Moulton, D.G. Patterned response to odor in single neurones of goldfish olfactory bulb: influence of odor quality and other stimulus parameters. J. of Gen. Physiol., 1978, 71, 615-643. 6. Goh, Y., Tamura, T. and Kobayashi, H. Olfactory responses to amino acids in marine teleosts. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 1979, 62A, 863-868. 7. Sutterlin, A.M. and Sutterlin, N. Electrical responses of the olfactory epithelium of atlantic Salmon (salmo salar). J. Fish. Res. Bd. Canada, 1971, 28, 565-572. 8. Suzuki, N. and Tucker, D. Amino acids as olfactory stimuli in fresh water catfish Ictalurus catus (Linn.). Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 1971, 40A, 399-404. 9. Tucker, D. and Suzuki, N. Olfactory responses to Schreckstoff of catfish. In: Schneider, D., Ed. "Olfaction and Taste IV." Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft, Stuttgart, 1972, 121-134. 10. Idler, D.R., Fagerlund, U.H. and Mayoh, H. Olfactory perception in migrating salmon I. L-serine, a salmon repellent in mammalian skin. J. Gen. Physiol., 1956, 39, 889-892. 11. Idler, D.R., McBride, J.R., Jonas, R.E.E. and Tomlinson, N. Olfactory perception in migrating salmon II. Studies on a laboratory bioassay for homestream water and mammalian repellent. Can. J. Biochem. Physiol., 1961, 39, 1575-1584. 12. Hamilton, P.B. Amino acids on hands. Nature, 1965, 205, 284-285. 13. Oro, J. and Skewes, H.B. Free amino acids on human fingers: the question of contamination in microanalysis. Nature, 1965, 207, 1042-1045. 14. Pawson, M.G. Analysis of a natural attractant for whiting Merlangius merlangus L. using a behavioral bioassay. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 1977, 56A, 129-135. 15. Konosu, S., Fusetani, N., Nose, T. and Hashimoto, Y. Attractants for eels in the extracts of short-necked clam - II. Survey of constitutents eliciting feeding behavior by fractionation of the extracts. Bull. Jap. Soc. Sci. Fish., 1968, 34, 84-87.

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

12.

MOULTON

Structure-Activity Relations in Olfaction

229

16. Hara, T.J. Olfaction in fish. Progress in Neurobiol., 1975, 5, 271-335. 17. Hara, T.J. Further studies on the structure-activity relationships of amino acids in fish olfaction. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 1977, 56A, 559-565. 18. MacLeod, N.K. Spontaneous activitiy of single neurons in the olfactory bulb of the rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri) and its modulation by olfactory stimulation with amino acids. Exp. Brain Res., 1976, 25, 267-278. 19. Caprio, J. Olfaction and taste in the channel catfish: an electrophysiological study of the responses to amino acids and derivatives. J. Comp. Physiol. A, 1978, 123, 357-371. 20. Doving, K.B., Selst, R. and Thommsen, G. Olfactory sensitivity to bile acids in salmonid fishes. Acta. Physiol. Scand., 1980, 108, 123-131. 21. Kauer, J.S. Response patterns of amphibian bulb neurones to odor stimulation. J. Physiol. (London), 1974, 243, 695-712. 22. Meredith, M. Olfactory coding: single unit response to amino acids in goldfish olfactory bulb. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Pennsylvania, 1974. 23. Meredith, M. Multidimensional scaling of olfactory bulb unit data. Presentation at 1st Meeting of Association for Chemo-reception, Sarasota, Florida, 1979. 24. Moulton, D.G. Studies in olfactory acuity 3. Relative detectability of n-aliphatic acetates by the rat. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 1960, 12, 203-213. 25. Schiffman, S.S. and Dachis, C. Taste of nutrients: amino acids, vitamins and fatty acids. Percept. and Psychophys., 1975, 17, 140-146. 26. Schiffman, S.S. and Engelhard, H.H. Taste of dipeptides. Physiol. and Beh., 1976, 17, 523-535. 27. Yoshida, M. Similarity of different kinds of taste near threshold concentration. Jap. J. Psychol., 1963, 34, 25-35. 28. Revial, M.F., Duchamp, A. and Holley, A. Odour discrimination by frog olfactory receptors: a second study. Chem. Senses and Flavour, 1978, 3, 7-21. 29. Revial, M.F., Duchamp, A., Holley, A. and MacLeod, P. Frog olfaction: odour groups, acceptor distribution and receptor categories. Chem. Senses and Flavour, 1978, 3, 23-33. 30. Getchell, T.V. Unitary responses in frog olfactory epithelium to sterically related molecules at low concentrations. J. Gen. Physiol., 1974, 64, 241-261. 31. Juge, A., Holley, A. and Delaleau, J.C. Olfactory receptor cell activity under electrical polarization of the nasal mucosa in the frog. J. Physiol.(Paris), 1979, 75, 919-927. 32. Baylin, F. and Moulton, D.G. Adaptation and cross-adaptation to odor stimulation of olfactory receptors in the tiger salamander. J. of Gen. Physiol., 1979, 74, 37-55. 33. Kubie, J.L. and Moulton, D.G. Regional patterning of re--

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.

230

34. 35.

Downloaded by UNIV OF MINNESOTA on October 14, 2014 | http://pubs.acs.org Publication Date: March 20, 1981 | doi: 10.1021/bk-1981-0148.ch012

36. 37. 38.

39. 40. 41. 42. 43.

44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49.

ODOR QUALITY AND CHEMICAL STRUCTURE

sponse to odors in the salamander olfactory mucosa. Neuroscience Absts., 1979, 5, 129. Mackay-Sim, A. and Moulton, D.G. Odorant specific maps of relative sensitivity inherent in the salamander olfactory epithelium. Neuroscience Absts. 6, 1980 (in press). Kauer, J.S. and Moulton, D.G. Responses of olfactory bulb neurones to odour stimulation of small nasal areas in the salamander. J. Physiol., 1974, 243, 717-737. Hornung, D.E. and Mozell, M.M. Factors influencing the differential sorption of odorant molecules across the olfactory mucosa. J. Gen. Physiol., 1977, 69, 343-361. Moulton, D.G. Spatial patterning of response to odors in the peripheral olfactory system. Physiol. Rev., 1976, 56, 578593. Talbot, W.H., Darian-Smith, I., Kornhuber, H.H. and Mountcastle, V.B. The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from monkey hand. J. Neurophysiol., 1968, 31, 301334. Green, D.G. and Siegel, I.M. Double-branched flicker fusion curves from the cone-rod skate retina. Science, 1975, 188, 1120-1122. Stevens, C.F. Biophysical analysis of the function of receptors. Ann. Rev. Physiol., 1980, 42, 643-652. Niccolini, P. Lo stimolo olfattorio et la sua recezione. Arch. Ital. Sci. Pharmacol., 1954, 4, 109-172. Getchell, T.V. and Getchell, M.L. Early events in vertebrate olfaction. Chem. Senses and Flavour, 1977, 2, 313-326. Gesteland, R.C. Receptor membrane functions in chemoreception: implications from single unit studies. In: Benz, G., Ed., "Structure-activity Relationships in Chemoreception." Information Retrieval, London, 1976, pp. 161-168. Baylin, F. Temporal patterns and selectivity in the unitary responses of olfactory receptors in the tiger salamander to odor stimulation. J. Gen. Physiol., 1979, 74, 17-36. Mathews, D.F. Response patterns of single neurones in the tortoise olfactory epithelium and bulb. J. Gen. Physiol., 1972, 60, 166-180. Mozell, M.M. Electrophysiology of the olfactory bulb. J. Neurophysiol., 1958, 21, 183-196. Moulton, D.G. and Marshall, D.A. The performance of dogs in detecting -ionone in the vapor phase. J. Comp. Physiol., 1976, 110, 287-306. Mathews, D.F. Rat olfactory nerve responses to odors. Chem. Senses and Flavour, 1974, 1, 69-76. Moulton, D.G. and Eayrs, J.T. Studies in olfactory acuity 2. Relative detectability of n-aliphatic alcohols by the rat. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 1960, 12, 99-109.

RECEIVED

October 13, 1980.

In Odor Quality and Chemical Structure; Moskowitz, H., et al.; ACS Symposium Series; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, 1981.